Since afterimages do not change in size at the retina,
what we see in the apparent changes of size noted by Emmert is conventionally regarded as a compensatory mechanism
at work. It is compensating for what would normally be changes of size of the
retinal image,
but as the afterimage remains unchanging, size constancy gives
illusory changes of size. The constancy
scaling is believed to be set by several 'distance cues' such as convergence of the eyes,
geometrical
perspective, and the graded texture and falling of sharpness ('aerial
perspective'), which are believed to be associated with increasing distance of a viewed
object. Correspondingly,
afterimages change their size with changes of
convergence angles of the eyes, or with perspective. Any cue to distance can set the brain's
perceptual compensatory size constancy scaling mechanism.
Emmert's law can be expressed by the following equation:
Perceived size = Retinal image size × Perceived distance.
Since we are already familiarized with McCready's work,
which has been discussed in many occasions in the
previous article, let's use his equation for Emmert's law:
S' =
D'·
θ, where
S' is the perceived linear size of an object;
D' is the perceived distance of the afterimage's projection;
and
θ is the retinal size of an afterimage. In the equation,
θ remains constant, and
S' will change depending on
D'.
As such, the perceived size of an afterimage is proportional to its perceived distance.
However,
a simple proportional relationship is deemed by many researchers to be
unlikely at very large distance. Therefore, the fact that Emmert's law
holds over a huge range of distance, which is seemingly far beyond what
size scaling can explain, leads to the conclusion that the law is still
not fully understood.
In the light of the monumental study of
Murray et al.,
which found that the perceived size of an object has
corresponding cortical activity in the primary visual cortex (V1), I
have done a simple calculation. If we say that the perceived size of an
afterimage is proportional to its perceived distance,
then doubling the distance would more than double the perceived
size. The reason is that the size used in the equation above is not the
full size of an afterimage, but merely the diameter or height.
Hence, the full size of an afterimage is proportional to the
perceived distance squared. Let's say that an afterimage occupies one
part out of a million in V1.
If that afterimage is projected onto a surface at a distance of
1000 units, the perceived full size of the afterimage would use up the
whole V1; and we could not perceive the projected afterimage anymore
beyond 1000 units because the perceived size of the afterimage has
already exceeded the capacity of V1.
However, our experience and various experiments tell us that we
still can perceive the afterimage at a distance far beyond 1000 units.
Thus the equation and the linear relationship between the
perceived size of an afterimage and the perceived distance are
demonstrated to be incredible.
The brain's
perceptual compensatory size constancy scaling mechanism is supposed to operate like this:
our brain 'projects' a retinal afterimage onto a surface on which the afterimage remains the same size. However,
the perceived size of the afterimage on a distant surface would
be perceived as smaller than before when it is reflected back onto the
retina if the original afterimage on the retina is projected onto the
distant surface unchanged.
This is incoherent because an afterimage on the retina should
not change size every time it is projected to a different distance.
Hence our brain uses the perceived distance based on various cues to
enlarge the perceived size of the afterimage to keep the retinal
afterimage constant.
If this explanation of Emmert's law is correct, then we should
be able to measure the increased perceived size of an afterimage with
increased distance, as shown in
S' =
D'·
θ.
To test this hypothesis, I set a ruler at a fixed distance from
my right eye, and then measured the perceived size of the afterimage,
obtained by staring at the black circle above for 20 or more seconds,
at different distances.
The outcome of measurements is that the perceived size of the
afterimage is exactly the same at different distances. Therefore, the
hypothesis is refuted and the equation and the conventional explanation
of Emmert's law are further discredited.
I think that the afterimage on
our retina is projected out just like the visual field as shown in the
diagram below.
I have envisioned the visual field spreading out like a
flashlight before. The projected afterimage of the black circle above
is literally like a flashlight when viewing it on a surface in a dark
room.
The projection of an afterimage can also be envisioned as a
movie projector. In the diagram below the Image is the perceived
afterimage. The thinner lines beside the Image are the edges of the
projection.
The size of the Image will not change no matter how far the
projection lines are extended because it is measured at a fixed spot on
the projection track.
The fundamental
distinction
between my diagram below and the conventional understanding of Emmert's
law is that the perceived size of an afterimage, I believe, is caused
by actual physical change of the afterimage on a surface, not by the
brain's processing system, the so-called compensatory size constancy
scaling mechanism.
As I said, if this mechanism or system exists, we should be able to measure the changes of the afterimage.
Richard Gregory believes that there is a change in retinal
stimulation
for each wall or screen lying at a different distance, for their images
will be different. But we cannot measure these image changes even if we
set the measuring instrument very close to the eye which is supposed to
be the measurement of the retinal size.
Likewise, we might not find any cortical activation change in V1
either when the projection of an afterimage is viewed through a fixed
converging point.
To further prove my point, I observed the afterimages through a paper tube.
The perceived size of an afterimage is always the same no
matter where the afterimage is projected onto. This result can be
depicted by the diagram below.
While viewing through the tube, the converging point is fixed at the
O position and the converging angle is fixed as well. As such the visual field volume is fixed at a certain distance.
For an afterimage to appear the same size at various distances,
it has to spread out in proportion to the visual field, as
shown in the diagram below. In so doing, the projected afterimage at
any distance viewed through the tube will have the same proportion in
the conical visual field.
Even though the absolute size of the afterimage changes with distance, it changes proportionally with the visual field.
As a result, the portion of an afterimage remains the same
within the visual field; and the perceived size of it will stay the
same as well.
This result has been confirmed by the study of Rock and
Ebenholtz (1959), which I have mentioned many times in the previous
articles. Observers in their study could not tell which central line
was longer,
as shown in the figure on the right, when they were shown the
rectangles separately, because the central lines in both rectangles have
the same proportion in the rectangles.
I believe that this is the only way that an afterimage can remain
the same size on the retina and in the cortex. Imagine that the visual
field is expanding as usual with distance and the projected afterimage
is the same at a longer distance.
When the projection is reflected back on the retina, its
retinal size will be smaller because it will occupy a smaller portion
of the retina space, and the situation is the same in the cortex.
I guess the basic rule is that the retinal size of an afterimage must stay the same by all means.
The best and only way to do so is to spread out the same way as
the visual field. Here the visual field is the determining factor which
causes the projected afterimage to expand with distance, and at the
same time to keep the afterimage staying the same on the retina.
The visual field volume instead of distance or perspective is the determining factor,
which I have stated many times earlier for the Delboeuf
illusion, as well as the Ponzo, Ebbinghaus, Ames Room illusions. Now I
claim that it is so for Emmert's law and later on for the Moon illusion.
To prove my point, I have designed a simple experiment, which
is inspired by the Delboeuf illusion figure. Stare at the two central
black circles below for about 20 seconds, then view their afterimages
on a dark wall or screen.
You will see two bright circles on the wall; the left
projected circle looks larger than the right one. As we have already
known, the black central circles are exactly the same size.
This outcome shows that the sub-visual field volume determines the perceived
size of a projected afterimage on a surface. Although the projected
afterimage grows with distance, the distance is not consequential for Emmert's law.
Otherwise, the afterimage would be perceived as larger when projected onto a distant surface viewed through a paper tube.
Or the projected bright circles would be the same on the wall.
More importantly,
to regard Emmert's law as a result of the same
proportion in the visual field, instead of the proportional increase of
an afterimage with the increased distance, we can avoid the consequence
of the afterimage filling up the visual cortex,
as discussed earlier. No matter how large the projected
afterimage will eventually grow with long distance, it still only
activates a portion of the visual cortex since it is a portion of the
visual field which might fill up the whole visual cortex at most.
Then,
if the retinal size of an afterimage is the same when projected
onto the surfaces of different distances and our brain does not
compensate for the shrinking effect of the distance, why do we all
perceive the afterimage as becoming larger when projected onto a more
distant wall or screen?
Well, here comes the second important concept, the converging
point, which I have used to explain the Ames Room illusion. As shown in
the diagram on the left below,
if we move the Focus1 forwards and place our converging point
at the Focus2, the sub-visual field will become much smaller for the
far object which in turn will be perceived as larger since it now
occupies a larger portion of the reduced visual field volume.
When we view the afterimages which are projected onto the
surfaces of different distances, we tend to focus on them. As a
consequence, we always place the converging point close to the
projected afterimages.
The expanding afterimage with distance will be perceived as
larger. The situation is similar to the picture on the right below.
(Now you have to imagine the scene in the real 3-D world rather than
the 2-D picture.
)
When viewed through a fixed converging point such as a camera
aperture, the back and front spheres are perceived as the same size if
the back sphere is five times bigger in diameter and five times further
away.
Likewise, the projections of an afterimage are perceived as the
same size while viewed through a tube. Instead of two positions in the
picture,
the projected afterimages are perceived as the same in any
position since they change in proportion to the visual field volume.
When you look at the back sphere without the camera, it appears to be
much bigger than the front sphere, which is similar to looking at the
afterimages on the wall without the tube.
Another
observation of Emmert's law is that when we project an afterimage onto
a nearer object such as a chair, the projected afterimage covers merely
a part of the chair;
but when the same chair is moved farther away, the projected
afterimage on that farther chair becomes much bigger than the chair.
This phenomenon can be explained by the diagram below.
The afterimage is expanding with the visual field, but the object is not. Therefore,
the afterimage becomes larger in comparison to the object as the
distance increases. The size of the afterimage is represented by the
two thinner diverging lines.
Previous studies about Emmert's law have reported that the perceived size of
an afterimage is smaller than predicted from the law at far distance.
If Emmert's law is defined as the linear increase of apparent
size with increasing distance, then we really have a problem here. In
order to save the conventional definition of Emmert's law,
some researchers hypothesized that the apparent
location of afterimage is in front of the screen, on which observers projected
the afterimage, at far distance. This
suggestion is reasonable but baseless. The afterimage is always thought
to be projected onto a solid surface; how could it be projected onto
the thin air? My explanation for this phenomenon is that it is due to
the minimum converging angle, as discussed in the
previous article.
When the minimum converging angle is reached, the converging point cannot be pushed forwards anymore.
Now the afterimage at a long distance, even though it is landed
on a solid surface, will be perceived as smaller since it occupies a
smaller portion of the visual field. This is the same as the situation
where we perceive a human figure as small as an ant at far distance
because the minimum converging angle does not let us focus our eyes
further on the figure.
Now I would like to discuss a study done by Broerse et al. (1992).
In that study, when observers projected afterimages of circular
patterns onto a surface slanting away from them, the images were
reported as being oval in shape.
But this did not occur when similar afterimages were projected
onto the slanting rear wall of an Ames Room. Instead of appearing as
ovals, the afterimages remained circular. They reasoned that it appears
as though the actually-slanted rear wall of the room not only looks as
if it were normal to the line of vision,
but also that it functions as if it were in such an orientation as far as a projection surface for afterimage is concerned.
However, my explanation is that the Ames Room is viewed through
a viewing hole; as such the afterimage is perceived as the same at any
distance as demonstrated by the experiment I have conducted with a
paper tube.
Hence the afterimages on the rear wall of the Ames Room remain
circular because the slanted wall or different distances will not
affect the perceived size of the afterimages. This result has nothing
to do with perceiving the rear wall of the Ames Room as being cubic.
On the other hand, when we can view the slanting surface
without a viewing hole, we can focus on the surface and hence bring the
converging point close to the surface.
Thus, the end of the circle that is projected onto the further
surface will appear bigger and wider than the other end that is
projected onto the nearer surface.
Based on our new understanding of the phenomenon,
Emmert's law should be defined as the proportional increase of
perceived size of an afterimage with increasing visual field volume.